What can you reliably predict about normal age related changes in cognitive functioning in middle adulthood?

Ackerman, P. L. (2000). Domain-specific knowledge as the “dark matter” of adult intelligence: Gf/Gc personality and interest correlates. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 55, P69P84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Ackerman, P. L., & Heggestad, E. D. (1997). Intelligence, personality, and interests: Evidence for overlapping traits. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 219245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Alwin, D. F. (2009). History, cohorts, and patterns of cognitive aging. In Bosworth, H. B. & Hertzog, C. (Eds.), Aging and cognition: Research methodologies and empirical advances (pp. 938). Washington: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Anstey, K. J., Hofer, S. M., & Luszcz, M. A. (2003). Cross-sectional and longitudinal patterns of dedifferentiation in late-life cognitive and sensory function: The effects of age, ability, attrition, and occasion of measurement. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 470487.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Bäckman, L., & Small, B. J. (2007). Cognitive deficits in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia: Patterns of findings from the Kungsholmen project. Physiology and Behavior, 92, 8086.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Ball, K., Berch, D. B., Helmer, K. F., Jobe, J. B., Leveck, M. D., Marsiske, M., et al. (2002). Effects of cognitive training interventions with older adults: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American Medical Association, 288, 22712281.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Baltes, P. B. (1997). On the incomplete architecture of human ontogeny: Selection, optimization, and compensation as a foundation for developmental theory. American Psychologist, 52, 366380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Baltes, P. B., & Labouvie, G. V. (1973). Adult development of intellectual performance: Description, explanation, and modification. In Eisdorfer, C. & Lawton, M. P. (Eds.), The psychology of adult development and aging (pp. 157219). Washington: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Baltes, P. B., & Nesselroade, J. R. (1970). Multivariate longitudinal and cross-sectional sequences for analyzing ontogenetic and generational change: A methodological note. Developmental Psychology, 2, 163168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Baltes, P. B., & Nesselroade, J. R. (1979). History and rationale of longitudinal research. In Nesselroade, J. R. & Baltes, P. B. (Eds.), Longitudinal research in the study of behavior and development. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar

Baltes, P. B., Staudinger, U. M., & Lindenberger, U. (1999). Lifespan psychology: Theory and application to intellectual functioning. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 471507.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Basak, C., Boot, W. R., Voss, M. W., & Kramer, A. F. (2008). Can training in a real-time strategy videogame attenuate cognitive decline in older adults? Psychology and Aging, 23, 765777.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Beier, M., & Ackerman, P. L. (2005). Age, ability, and the role of prior knowledge on the acquisition of new domain knowledge: Promising results in a real-world learning environment. Psychology and Aging, 20, 341355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Berg, S. (1996). Aging, behavior, and terminal decline. In Birren, J. E. & Schaie, K. W. (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (4th ed., pp. 323337). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar

Birren, J. E. (1964). The psychology of aging. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar

Bosworth, H. B., Schaie, K. W., & Willis, S. L. (1999). Cognitive and sociodemographic risk factors for mortality in the Seattle Longitudinal Study. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 54, P273P282.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Botwinick, J. (1977). Intellectual abilities. In Birren, J. E. & Schaie, K. W. (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (pp. 580605). New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.Google Scholar

Brickley, P. G., Keith, T. Z., & Wolfle, L. M. (1995). The three-stratum theory of cognitive abilities: Test of the structure of intellect across the adult life span. Intelligence, 20, 309328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Buchman, A. S., Tanne, D., Boyle, P. A., Shah, R. C., Leurgans, S. E., & Bennett, D. A. (2009). Kidney function is associated with the rate of cognitive decline in the elderly. Neurology, 73, 920927.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Carlson, M. C., Saczynski, J. S., Rebok, G. W., McGill, S., Tielsch, J., Glass, T. A., et al. (2008). Exploring the effects of an everyday activity program on executive function and memory in older adults: Experience Corps. The Gerontologist, 48, 793801.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities: A survey of factor analytic studies. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Cattell, R. B. (1971). Abilities: Their structure, growth, and action. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar

Charness, N. (1981). Aging and skilled problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 110, 2138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Cianciolo, A. T., Matthew, C., Sternberg, R. J., & Wagner, R. K. (2006). Tacit knowledge, practical intelligence, and expertise. In Ericsson, K. A., Charness, N., Feltovich, P. J., & Hoffman, R. R. (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance (pp. 613632). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Colcombe, S., & Kramer, A. F. (2003). Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Science, 14, 125130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Colonia-Willner, R. (1998). Practical intelligence at work: Relationships between aging and cognitive efficiency among managers in a bank environment. Psychology and Aging, 13, 4557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Czaja, S., Charness, N., Fisk, A. D., Hertzog, C., Nair, S., Rogers, W. A., et al. (2006). Factors predicting the use of technology: Findings from the Center for Research and Education on Aging and Technology Enhancement (CREATE). Psychology and Aging, 21, 333352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Deary, I. J., Whiteman, M. C., Starr, J. M., Whalley, L. J., & Fox, H. C. (2004). The impact of childhood intelligence on later life: Following up the Scottish Mental Surveys of 1932 and 1947. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 130147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

deFrias, C. M., Lövdén, M., Lindenberger, U., & Nilsson, L-G. (2007). Revisiting the de-differentiation hypothesis with longitudinal multi-cohort data. Intelligence, 35, 381392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Eggermont, L. H. P., Milberg, W. P., Lipsitz, L. A., Scherder, E. J. A., & Leveille, S. G. (2009). Physical activity and executive function in aging: The MOBILIZE Boston study. Journal of the American Geriatric Society, 57, 17501756.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Ferrer, E., Salthouse, T. A., McArdle, J. J., Stewart, W. F., & Schwartz, B. S. (2005). Multivariate modeling of age and retest in longitudinal studies of cognitive abilities. Psychology and Aging, 20, 412422.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Ferrer, E., Salthouse, T. A., Stewart, W. F., & Schwartz, B. S. (2004). Modeling age and retest processes in longitudinal studies of cognitive abilities. Psychology and Aging, 19, 243249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Flynn, J. R. (2007). What is intelligence? Beyond the Flynn effect. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Fox, M. C., Berry, J. M., & Freeman, S. P. (2014). Are vocabulary tests measurement invariant between age groups? An item response analysis of three popular tests. Psychology and Aging, 29, 925938.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Fox, M. C., & Mitchum, A. L. (2013). A knowledge-based theory of rising scores on “culture free“ tests. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 9791000.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Ghisletta, P., McArdle, J. J., & Lindenberger, U. (2006). Longitudinal cognition-survival relations in old and very old age: 13-year data from the Berlin Aging Study. European Psychologist, 11, 204223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Hall, C. B., Lipton, R. B., Sliwinski, M., & Stewart, W. F. (2000). A change point model for estimating the onset of cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Statistics in Medicine, 19, 15551566.3.0.CO;2-3>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Hambrick, D. Z., Meinz, E. J., & Salthouse, T. A. (1999). Predictors of crossword puzzle proficiency and moderators of age-cognition relations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 131164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Harris, S. E., Fox, H., Wright, A. F., Hayward, C., Starr, J. M., Whalley, L. J., Deary, I. J. (2006). The brain-derived neurotrophic factor Val66Met polymorphism is associated with age-related change in reasoning skills. Molecular Psychiatry, 11, 505513.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Hershey, D. A., Jacobs-Lawson, J. M., & Walsh, D. A. (2003). Influences of age and training on script development. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 10, 119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Hertzog, C. (1989). The influence of cognitive slowing on age differences in intelligence. Developmental Psychology, 25, 636651.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Hertzog, C. (2008). Theoretical approaches to the study of cognitive aging: An individual-differences perspective. In Hofer, S. M. & Alwin, D. F. (Eds.), Handbook of cognitive aging: Interdisciplinary perspectives (pp. 3449). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Hertzog, C. (2009). Use it or lose it: An old hypothesis, new evidence, and an ongoing controversy. In Bosworth, H. & Hertzog, C. (Eds.), Cognition and aging: Research methodologies and empirical advances (pp. 161179). Washington: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Hertzog, C., & Bleckley, M. K. (2001). Age differences in the structure of intelligence: Influences of information processing speed. Intelligence, 29, 191217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Hertzog, C., Dixon, R. A., Hultsch, D. F., & MacDonald, S. W. S. (2003). Latent change models of adult cognition: Are changes in processing speed and working memory associated with changes in episodic memory? Psychology and Aging, 18, 755769.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Hertzog, C., Kramer, A. F., Wilson, R. S., & Lindenberger, U. (2008). Enrichment effects on adult cognitive development: Can the functional capacity of older adults be preserved and enhanced? Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 9, 165.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Hertzog, C., & Schaie, K. W. (1986). Stability and change in adult intelligence: 1. Analysis of longitudinal covariance structures. Psychology and Aging, 1, 159171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Hofer, S. M., Flaherty, B. P., & Hoffman, L. (2006). Cross-sectional analysis of time-dependent data: Mean-induced association in age-heterogeneous samples and an alternative method based on sequential narrow age-cohort samples. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 41, 165187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Horn, J. L. (1985). Remodeling old models of intelligence: Gf–Gc theory. In Wolman, B. B. (Ed.), Handbook of intelligence (pp. 267300). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar

Horn, J. L., & Cattell, R. B. (1967). Age differences in fluid and crystallized intelligence. Acta Psychologica, 26, 107129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Horn, J. L., Donaldson, G., & Engstrom, R. (1981). Apprehension, memory, and fluid intelligence decline in adulthood. Research on Aging, 3, 3384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Horn, J. L., & Hofer, S. M. (1992). Major abilities and development in the adult period. In Sternberg, R. J. & Berg, C. A. (Eds.), Intellectual development (pp. 4499). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar

Hultsch, D. F., Hertzog, C., Dixon, R. A., & Small, B. J. (1998). Memory change in the aged. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar

Hultsch, D. F., Small, B. J., Hertzog, C., & Dixon, R. A. (1999). Use it or lose it: Engaged lifestyle as a buffer of cognitive decline in aging. Psychology and Aging, 14, 245263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2002). The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: An individual differences perspective. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9, 637671.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Kramer, A. F., & Colcombe, S. (2018). Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: A meta-analytic study – revisited. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13, 213217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Kyllonen, P. C., & Chrystal, R. E. (1990). Reasoning ability is (little more than) working-memory capacity? Intelligence, 14, 389433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Lane, C. J., & Zelinski, E. M. (2003). Longitudinal hierarchical linear models of the Memory Functioning Questionnaire. Psychology and Aging, 18, 3853.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Laukka, E. J., MacDonald, S. M. S., & and Bäckman, L. (2008). Terminal-decline effects for select cognitive tasks after controlling for preclinical dementia. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 16, 355365.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Lindenberger, U., Nagel, I. E., Chicherio, C., Li, S-C., Heekeren, H. R., & Bäckman, L. (2008). Age-related decline in brain resources modulates genetic effects on cognitive functioning. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 2, 234244.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Lindenberger, U., von Oertzen, T., Ghisletta, P., & Hertzog, C. (2011). Cross-sectional age variance extraction: What’s change got to do with it? Psychology and Aging, 26, 3447.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Lövdén, M., Rönnlund, M., Wahlin, A., Bäckman, L., Nyberg, L., & Goran-Nilsson, L. (2004). The extent of stability and change in episodic and semantic memory in old age: Demographic predictors of stability and change. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 59B, P130P134.Google Scholar

MacKinnon, A., Christensen, H., Hofer, S. M., Korten, A. E., & Jorm, A. F. (2003). Use it and still lose it? The association between activity and cognitive performance established using latent growth techniques in a community sample. Aging Neuropsychology and Cognition, 10, 215222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Masunaga, H., & Horn, J. L. (2001). Expertise and age-related changes in components of intelligence. Psychology and Aging, 16, 293311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

McArdle, J. J., & Bell, R. Q. (2001). An introduction to latent growth models for developmental data analysis. In Little, T. D. & Schabel, K. U. (Eds.), Modeling longitudinal and multi-level data: Practical issues, applied approaches, and specific examples (pp. 6981). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar

McArdle, J. J., Ferrer-Caja, E., Hamagami, F., & Woodcock, R. W. (2002). Comparative longitudinal structural analyses of the growth and decline of multiple intellectual abilities over the life span. Developmental Psychology, 38, 115142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Meredith, W., & Horn, J. L. (2001). The role of factorial invariance in modeling growth and change. In Collins, L. M. & Sayer, A. G. (Eds.), New methods for the analysis of change (pp. 203240). Washington: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Ng, T. W. H., & Feldman, D. C. (2008). The relationship of age to ten dimensions of job performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93, 392423.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Nilsson, L-G., Sternäng, O., Rönnlund, M., & Nyberg, L. (2009). Challenging the notion of an early onset of cognitive decline. Neurobiology of Aging, 30, 521524.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Park, D. C., Hertzog, C., Leventhal, H., Morrell, R. W., Leventhal, E., Birchmore, D., et al. (1999). Medication adherence in rheumatoid arthritis patients: Older is wiser. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 47, 172183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Park, D. C., Smith, A. D., Lautenschlager, G., Earles, J. L., Frieske, D., Zwahr, M., &Gaines, C. L. (1996). Mediators of long-term memory performance across the life span. Psychology and Aging, 11, 621637.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Phillips, L. H., Henry, J. D., & Martin, M. (2008). Adult aging and prospective memory: The importance of ecological validity. In Kliegel, M., McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (Eds.), Prospective memory: Cognitive, neuroscience, developmental, and applied perspectives (pp. 161185). New York: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar

Pietschnig, J., & Voracek, M. (2015). One century of global IQ gains: A formal meta-analysis of the Flynn effect (1909–2013). Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10, 282315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Rabbitt, P. (1993). Does it all go together when it goes? The nineteenth Bartlett memorial lecture. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46A, 385434.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Rabbitt, P., Diggle, P., Holland, F., & McInnes, L. (2004). Practice and drop-out effects during a 17-year longitudinal study of cognitive aging. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 59B, P84P97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Ram, N., Gerstorf, D., Fauth, E., Zarit, S., & Malmberg, B. (2010). Aging, disablement, and dying: Using time-as-process and time-as-resources metrics to chart late-life change. Research on Human Development, 7, 2744.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Raven, J. (2000). The Raven’s Progressive Matrices: Change and stability over culture and time. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Raz, N., Lindenberger, U., Ghisletta, P., Rodrigue, K. M., Kennedy, K. M., & Acker, J. M. (2008). Neuroanatomical correlates of fluid intelligence in healthy adults and persons with vascular risk factors. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 718726.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Reynolds, C. A. (2008). Genetic and environmental influences on cognitive change. In Hofer, S. M. & Alwin, D. F. (Eds.), Handbook of cognitive aging: Interdisciplinary perspectives (pp. 557574). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Rönnlund, M., & Nilsson, L-G. (2008). The magnitude, generality, and determinants of Flynn effects on forms of declarative memory and visuospatial ability: Time-sequential analyses of data from a Swedish cohort study. Intelligence, 36, 192209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Rönnlund, M., Nyberg, L., Bäckman, L., & Nilsson, L-G. (2005). Stability, growth, and decline in adult life span development of declarative memory: Data from a population-based study. Psychology and Aging, 20, 318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Salthouse, T. A. (1982). Adult cognition: An experimental psychology of human aging. New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review, 103, 403428.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Salthouse, T. A. (2006). Mental exercise and mental aging: Evaluating the validity of the “use it or lose it” hypothesis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 6887.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Salthouse, T. A., Pink, J. E., & Tucker-Drob, E. M. (2008). Contextual analysis of fluid intelligence. Intelligence, 36, 464486.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Schaie, K. W. (1977). Quasi-experimental designs in the psychology of aging. In Birren, J. E. & Schaie, K. W. (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (pp. 3958). New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.Google Scholar

Schaie, K. W. (1989). Perceptual speed in adulthood: Cross-sectional and longitudinal studies. Psychology and Aging, 4, 443453.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Schaie, K. W. (2009). “When does age-related cognitive decline begin?”: Salthouse again reifies the “cross-sectional fallacy.” Neurobiology of Aging, 30, 528529.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Schaie, K. W. (2012). Developmental influences on adult intelligence: The Seattle Longitudinal Study (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Schaie, K. W., Maitland, S. B., Willis, S. L, & Intrieri, R. C. (1998). Longitudinal invariance of adult psychometric ability factor structures across 7 years. Psychology and Aging, 13, 820.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Schooler, C., Mulatu, M. S., & Oates, G. (1999). The continuing effects of substantively complex work on the intellectual functioning of older workers. Psychology and Aging, 14, 483506.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Shadish, W., Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (2002). Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for generalized causal inference. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar

Singer, T., Verhaeghen, P., Ghisletta, P., Lindenberger, U., & Baltes, P. B. (2003). The fate of cognition in very old age: Six-year longitudinal findings in the Berlin Aging Study (BASE). Psychology and Aging, 18, 318331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Sliwinski, M., & Buschke, H. (2004). Modeling intraindividual cognitive change in aging adults: Results from the Einstein Aging Studies. Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 11, 196211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Sliwinski, M. J., Hofer, S. M., Hall, C., Bushke, H., & Lipton, R. B. (2003). Modeling memory decline in older adults: The importance of preclinical dementia, attrition and chronological age. Psychology and Aging, 18, 658671.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Sliwinski, M. J., Hoffman, L., & Hofer, S. M. (2010). Evaluating convergence of within-person change and between-person differences in age-heterogeneous longitudinal studies. Research on Human Development, 7, 4560.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Spiro, A. III, & Brady, C. B. (2008).Integrating health into cognitive aging research and theory: Quo vadis? In, S. M. Hofer & Alwin, D. F. (Eds.), Handbook of cognitive aging: Interdisciplinary perspectives (pp. 260283). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Sternberg, R. J. (1985). Beyond IQ: A triarchic theory of human intelligence. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar

Stine-Morrow, A. L., Parisi, J. M., Morrow, D. G., Greene, J., & Park, D. C. (2007). The senior odyssey project: A model of intellectual and social engagement. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 62B, P62P69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Thorvaldsson, V., Hofer, S. M., Berg, S., Skoog, I., Sacuiu, S., & Johansson, B. (2008). Onset of terminal decline in cognitive abilities in individuals without dementia. Neurology, 71, 882887.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Thurstone, L. L. (1938). Primary mental abilities. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar

Tranter, L. J., & Koutstaal, W. (2008). Age and flexible thinking: An experimental demonstration of the beneficial effects of increased cognitively stimulating activity on fluid intelligence in healthy older adults. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 15, 184207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Tucker-Drob, E. M. (2011). Global and domain-specific changes in cognition throughout adulthood. Developmental Psychology, 47, 331343.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Verhaeghen, P., & Salthouse, T. A.(1997). Meta-analyses of age-cognition relations in adulthood: Estimates of linear and non-linear age effects and structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 231249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Wechsler, D. (1939). Measurement of adult intelligence. Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Wilson, R. S., Beck, T. L., Bienias, J. L., & Bennett, D. A. (2007). Terminal cognitive decline: Accelerated loss of cognition in the last years of life. Psychosomatic Medicine, 69, 131137.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Wilson, R. S., Bennett, D. A., Bienias, J. L., Mendes de Leon, C. F., Morris, M. C., & Evans, D. A. (2003). Cognitive activity and cognitive decline in a biracial community population. Neurology, 61, 812816.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Zelinski, E. M., & Kennison, R. F. (2007). Not your father’s test scores: Cohort reduces psychometric aging effects. Psychology and Aging, 22, 546557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Zelinski, E. M., Kennison, R. F., Watts, A., & Lewis, K. L. (2009). Convergence between cross-sectional and longitudinal studies: Cohort matters. In Bosworth, H. B. & Hertzog, C. (Eds.), Aging and cognition: Research methodologies and empirical advances (pp. 101118). Washington: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar