What is the name of the structured observational procedure that reveals the security of attachment when the child is placed under stress?

Studies have demonstrated a strong relation between adult attachment security, using the Adult Attachment Interview (AAI), and infant security, using the Strange Situation Procedure (SSP). This suggests that a mother’s representations of attachment may influence the development of her infant’s attachment to her. This study both confirms and modifies that finding in a cohort of 47 first-time mothers and their infants. The AAIs were administered during the third trimester of pregnancy and the SSPs were performed when the infant was 14 months of age. The AAIs were classified using Crittenden’s Dynamic-Maturational Model (DMM) and the SSPs using both the DMM and also Main and Solomon’s ABC+D methods. There was a significant match of patterns for secure mothers and babies, but a tendency for inversion of insecure patterns of attachment, i.e., Type A mothers often had infants with a Type C pattern and vice versa. No significant relation was seen between the DMM adult and ABC+D infant patterns of attachment. A significant, but modest, association was found between the DMM and ABC+D infant SSP classifications. These findings may help guide treatment of insecure mother-infant dyads by individualizing interventions to include a focus on maternal representations of the infant and maternal responses to infant behavior.

Keywords: AAI, Attachment, DMM, Intergenerational, Transmission

A central hypothesis in Main’s approach to attachment theory is that mothers’ mental representations of attachment relationships are an important predictor of infants’ quality of attachment (Main, Kaplan, & Cassidy, 1985). That is, patterns of sensitive responsiveness appear to be intergenerationally transmitted, as assessed through the Adult Attachment Interview (AAI, George, Kaplan, & Main, 1985), and the Strange Situation Procedure (SSP, Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978).

However, evidence for continuity of attachment from mother to infant is more robust for secure attachment than for anxious (insecure) attachment. In a meta-analysis of 661 dyads in 13 studies, van IJzendoorn (1995) found that 75% of mothers and infants had matching secure versus insecure classifications. However, when a 4-way classification (secure, avoidant, ambivalent, and disorganized) was tested, mother-infant agreement dropped to 63% with even less agreement seen for insecure attachment groups. This suggests that the intergenerational transmission of attachment may be more complex than a direct replication of the mother’s pattern, and that the patterns of infant and adult attachment might be multi-dimensional. We explored these possibilities in a longitudinal study of first-time mothers on whom we have prenatal maternal representations and infant-mother attachment at 14 months. We address both conceptual and methodological issues to understand whether and how patterns of attachment are ‘transmitted’ from mothers to their infants.

Following Ainsworth’s groundbreaking work in the 1970’s establishing a predictive association between maternal sensitive responsiveness during the first year of life and infant-mother attachment at eleven months (Ainsworth, et al., 1978), attention turned to attachment in risk populations and at older ages. In the early reports, many maltreated infants (cf. Crittenden, 1985; Egeland & Sroufe, 1981) and 13% of infants in low-risk samples (Main, & Solomon, 1990) were found to be unclassifiable in the SSP according to Ainsworth’s original classificatory method. In both cases, there appeared to be an over-assignment of infants to Type B, secure attachment. To resolve this problem, two of Ainsworth’s students, Mary Main and Patricia Crittenden, expanded Ainsworth’s classificatory system by developing new categories and coding guidelines. The differences in their approaches permit us to explore the intergenerational transmission of attachment in greater detail than in previous studies.

Continuity-and-disorganization

The better-known approach is Main’s addition of a fourth category called ‘disorganized’ attachment in infancy (Type D) (Main & Solomon, 1990) and ‘unresolved’ and ‘cannot classify’ in adulthood (Hesse, 1996; Main, 2000). Main proposed that having a frightened or frightening mother made it impossible for the infant to organize a coherent pattern of attachment (cf. Main, 1995, 1996). Disorganization was initially described in a normative sample of 12-month-old infants who demonstrated conflicted or anomalous behavior during reunions in the Strange Situation (Main & Solomon, 1990).

For Main, the infant’s anomalous behavior reflected the lack of a strategy to manage fear associated with the caregiver’s frightening behavior: the infant’s fear could not be deactivated by a shift in attention (Ainsworth’s A (avoidant) pattern), nor could it be ameliorated though approaching the caregiver (Ainsworth’s B (secure) and C (ambivalent) patterns). Furthermore, Main theorized that the caregiver’s frightening behavior was connected with a history of unresolved loss or trauma (Main & Hesse, 1990).

The parent’s representational processes and response to memories became a central focus of Main’s subsequent work in attachment. The Adult Attachment Interview (George, et al., 1985) was developed to ascertain caregivers’ states of mind with respect to attachment. The coding system for the AAI was constructed to maximize the correspondence between AAI classifications and infant’s pre-existing SSP classifications based on Main’s beliefs in (1) the ‘transmission’ of attachment from mother to infant and (2) continuity of attachment across the lifespan (Main, et al., 1985; Main, 2000). The coding method was developed by searching for commonalities in the interview responses of mothers whose infants shared the same attachment classification. Consequently, in the AAI coding system (Main & Goldwyn, 1985–1994), the array of classifications was almost the same as in infancy albeit relabeled. Blind coding of AAI interviews using the Main and Goldwyn method (M&G-AAI) matched Strange Situation coding of security versus insecurity in 75% of cases (Main, et al., 1985). In addition, following Bowlby (1980), Main proposed that each individual had one enduring ‘inner working model’ (IWM) of attachment. When individuals were not able to form a single model, or they oscillated among models, it was treated as disorganization. We label this approach which is focused on continuity and disorganization as ‘ABC+D’.

Dynamic-Maturational Model (DMM)

Concurrent with the development of the continuity-and-disorganization expansion of Ainsworth’s work, Crittenden developed an expansion predicated on different ideas. She believed that, with maturation of the brain, more sophisticated strategies could develop in a dynamic interaction with on-going experience; these strategies were described in a series of papers (e.g. Crittenden, 1981, 1985, 2008; Crittenden & Ainsworth, 1989). In Crittenden’s model, patterns of attachment were considered to be ‘self-protective strategies’ that varied dimensionally (rather than categorically) in terms of the relative use of cognitive-contingent information or affect-arousing information to organize behavior. Furthermore, each individual was thought to have multiple representations based on different processing pathways (i.e., memory systems, Schacter & Tulving, 1994); these were called ‘dispositional representations’ (DRs), with the possibility that different DRs could regulate behavior under different conditions. The degree of integration among the DRs was the other dimension in her model.

Crittenden expected threatened children and adults to show organized attachment strategies that reflected more complex Type A and C organizations than Ainsworth’s A1–2, B, and C1–2 patterns. The new patterns were numbered A3–8 and C3–8 and included organized A/C combinations (Crittenden, 1985; Radke-Yarrow, Cummings, Kuczynski, & Chapman, 1985). There was no ‘disorganized’ category and, in fact, fear was treated as a powerful organizing affect (cf., Ledoux, 1996). This differs from Main, who considered fear a disorganizing mechanism (Main & Hesse, 1990).

Further, events occurring after infancy could affect a person’s strategy; that is, attachment was biased toward constancy, but could change if the environment changed (Crittenden, 2000; Hamilton, 2000; Lewis, Fearing, & Rosenthal, 2000; Weinfeld, Sroufe, & Egeland, 2000; Thompson & Raikes, 2003). Because childbirth, especially the birth of a first child, is a major transforming life event, it would be expected that there would be some discontinuity in mothers’ representations of attachment before and after birth.

In the DMM, patterns of attachment are conceptualized as self-protective strategies that are learned through interaction with attachment figures. The learning itself is based on both temporal information (termed “cognition”) and the intensity of stimulation (or “affect”), as these are processed through various parts of the brain. That is, information processing yields dispositional representations that, in turn, organize self-protective behavioral strategies. Infant behavior of all types is presumed to be a functional aspect of the infant-caregiver relationship and to maximize the infant’s probability of survival, given the caregiver’s behavior (Crittenden, 2008). In the DMM, information that requires little transformation to yield protective behavior (Type B strategy) promotes balanced relationships, whereas information that must be distorted to yield protective behavior (Types A, C, and A/C) promotes anxious relationships. When the mother is sensitively responsive (B), the infant is expected to be secure (B). When the mother is unpredictably, intermittently responsive to the child’s negative affect (especially anger, fear and desire for comfort), as is seen in a Type A pattern of responsiveness, the infant may utilize a reverse strategy (e.g., an affect-laden Type C strategy) to reach an affectively distant mother. One would expect to fine a relatively few Type C infants because a Type C strategy cannot be organized around changing contingencies (Crittenden & Claussen, 2000). The reverse is expected if the mother demonstrates a Type C pattern of unpredictable responsiveness. For example, if the mother is mildly disengaged and ignores the infant’s mild displays of negative affect (e.g. mother displays C1–2 behavior), the infant learns that his displays of negative affect alienate his caregiver, and predict maternal rejection. Thus, the infant will learn to ignore his internal feeling states, and will consistently inhibit his expression of negative affect, and demonstrate behavior consistent with A1–2 strategy. This theory is called the Dynamic-Maturation Model of attachment and adaptation (DMM).

Comparing the models

Because they are both based on Ainsworth’s work, the Main and Crittenden models have much in common. Both use Ainsworth’s ABC structure, share belief in the effects of maternal behavior on infant organization, expect continuity of mother-infant patterning in cases of security, and expect the newer patterns to be associated with risk. Furthermore, both models recognize the role of fear in shaping the infant’s subsequent attachment strategy, and crucially, both use the same observational procedures (SSP and AAI). However, differences between the two classification systems include (1) whether exposure to danger (and consequent fear) disorganizes or organizes behavior, and (2) whether the distinction between security and insecurity is a clear categorical difference, or whether the distinction between the two is more subtle, varying dimensionally. Though DMM still employs distinct categories (e.g. A1–4, child patterns, and A1–8, adult patterns), these are ordered and progressive in nature, providing a dimensional quality to the DMM coding system. These differences between models facilitate comparison and testing of differentiating hypotheses.

In the face of parental danger (e.g. maltreatment), Main and Crittenden interpret the infant’s behavior in different ways. Main proposes that the infant’s “disorganized” behavior is the means by which the infant tries to manage his fear of his parent. For Main, the “disorganized” infant experiences the parent as the source of fear, and is faced with an irresolvable paradox in which the parent is both the means of protection and the source of alarm (Main & Hesse, 1990). Conversely, Crittenden posits that the infant’s behavior in the face of danger is an organizing strategy to foster self-protection. Crittenden further suggests that the infant’s behavioral strategies will vary based on the caregiver’s behavior and which infant memory system is activated most strongly (Crittenden, 1999).

In addition, though both Main and Crittenden believe that Type B, “secure” attachment is the most balanced, and the least vulnerable to pathology (Main, 2000; Crittenden, 2006), the two models approach the process of becoming securely attached differently. For Main, security of attachment is predictable from the mother’s sensitivity to the infant’s signals and is clearly identifiable by the infant’s organized ability to seek proximity to the mother when distressed, and to engage in exploration of the environment when not distressed (Main, 2000). In Main’s (ABC+D) classification, the distinction between security and insecurity was sharply defined by categorical differences in infant behavior. In contrast, Crittenden posited that the infant develops this organized strategy through a process of integration of cognitive and affective information. These two dimensions involve (1) a relative reliance on one or the other of two types of information (affect or cognition) and (2) a degree of integration of the information and corresponding representations. Infants who use a cognitive (Type A) strategy, organize around expectations of rejection of displays of negative affect, whereas infants who use an affective (Type C) strategy organize experiences around their feelings. Crittenden posits that infants use both Type A and Type C strategies (Crittenden, 2006). In this regard, in the DMM, the distinction between security and insecurity is not as sharply delineated as in a categorical (ABC+D) model, but rather varies more subtly, along a dimensional continuum.

Other areas of difference between the Main and Crittenden models include (3) whether early conditions largely determine later outcomes (developmental trajectory) or experience has a cumulative and dynamic effect (developmental pathways), (4) whether individuals have a single enduring IWM or multiple, situation-specific DRs, and (5) whether a 4-category model based on infancy can describe attachment at later ages or additional patterns are needed. With regard to mother-to-infant transmission of attachment patterns or strategies, the differences are whether continuity is to be expected in all cases or, conversely, whether (a) some mothers will change pattern, e.g., after childbirth and (b) some infants will organize the opposite pattern from their mothers.

To date, only three studies have compared the ABC+D and DMM models (Crittenden, Claussen, & Kozlowska, 2007; Crittenden & Newman, this issue; Spieker & Crittenden, 2009). However, no studies have compared the two models in infancy, and only one study (Hautamäki, Hautamäki, Neuvonen, & Maliniemi-Piispanen, in press) has used the DMM method to test the intergenerational transmission of attachment. If an intergenerational relation from mother to infant were found using the DMM, it would strengthen the argument for continuity because the DMM-AAI is not predicated upon continuity, but rather upon adult adaptation. Furthermore, if there were predictable deviations away from continuity, it would expand our understanding of human adaptation. In this study we used data derived from both models to explore intergenerational transmission of attachment.

This study addressed the following hypotheses:

  1. Within secure attachment, there will be evidence of continuity between mother and infant attachment classifications (Type B mothers → Type B infants) which we will describe as matching of attachment classifications.

  2. Within insecure attachment, there will be some evidence of classification inversion between generations, wherein anxiously attached mothers will have infants who use the opposite anxious strategy (Type A mothers→ Type C infants and vice versa); this reversal of insecure attachment patterns between generations will be described as meshing.

  3. DMM-SSP classifications will be related, but not identical, to ABC+D-SSP classifications;

  4. DMM-AAI classifications will be associated with ABC+D-SSP classifications, albeit less strongly than between the two DMM methods.

We recruited first-time pregnant women in Houston, Texas during the third trimester to participate in an event-related functional MRI (fMRI) study to determine how mothers’ brains respond to their own infants’ facial expressions (Strathearn, Li, Fonagy, & Montague, 2008; Strathearn, Fonagy, Amico, & Montague, 2009). Recruitment occurred through prenatal clinic visits and advertisements on billboards, in magazines, and via the internet. We excluded potential subjects who were on psychotropic medications, used cigarettes during pregnancy, were left-handed, or had any contraindications to MRI scanning. Research was approved by the Institutional Review Board at Baylor College of Medicine, and all subjects provided written informed consent.

Of 112 women, 61 met eligibility criteria and were enrolled in the study with 49 having complete AAI and SSP classifications. The expectant mothers’ median age was 27.8 years (range 19–41). The majority was married (70.5%), Caucasian (62%), and college educated (79%), with a range of incomes from <$15,000/year to >$100,000/year; the median WTAR-predicted IQ for the group was 110 (range: 81–120). There were no differences between those who participated in the 14-month visit, and those who were enrolled, but were later lost to follow-up. See Strathearn, et al. (2009) for further details.

This was a longitudinal design with four visits over approximately 16 months.

Visit 1: Pregnancy

During this visit, we collected socio-demographic data, administered the AAI, Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI, Beck, Steer, & Brown, 1996), and Personality Disorder Questionnaire 4+ (PDQ, Hyler, Skodol, Oldham, Kellman, & Doidge, 1992). We repeated the BDI at each post-natal visit.

Visit 2: 7 months

At approximately 7 months post-delivery, each mother-infant dyad attended a session at the Human Neuroimaging Laboratory at Baylor College of Medicine. At this time, the mothers completed the Parenting Stress Index (PSI, Abidin, 1995), Adult Temperament Questionnaire-Short Form (ATQ, Rothbart, Ahadi, & Evans, 2000), and the Infant Behavior Questionnaire – Revised (IBR, Gartstein & Rothbart, 2003). Mothers also reported their breastfeeding status which was repeated at visit 3.

Visit 3: 11 months

At 11 months post-delivery, each mother underwent fMRI scanning while viewing 60 unique infant face images, 30 of her own baby and 30 of a matched unknown infant face, as previously described (Strathearn, et al., 2008, 2009).

Visit 4: 14 months

At 14 months of age, we assessed the infant using the Screening Test of the Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development (Bayley-III, Bayley 2006) and the Strange Situation Procedure (Ainsworth, et al. 1978).

Maternal attachment

Mothers’ attachment classifications were based on the AAI. The AAI is a semi-structured interview, consisting of 18 questions, which characterizes a mother’s capacity to form secure attachment relationships based on the narrative of her own childhood attachment experiences with her parents. The modified AAI that we used included additional questions designed to probe for six memory systems (DMM-AAI, Crittenden & Landini, in press, see Farnfield, Hautamäki, Nørbech, & Sahhar, this issue).

The interviews were audio-recorded, transcribed verbatim, and then coded by research-reliable coders, blinded to other aspects of the study, based on the subject’s coherence and consistency in describing attachment related experiences (Crittenden, 1999; Crittenden & Landini, in press). Sixty percent of the transcripts were double coded to assess inter-coder agreement, with 82% agreement on a 4-group (A, B, C, and A/C) classification (kappa = 0.74). Inter-coder agreement for 2-groups (B and non-B) was 89% (kappa = 0.78). Discrepancies were resolved through conferencing between coders.

Infant-mother attachment

Infant pattern of attachment was assessed using Ainsworth’s SSP (Ainsworth, et al. 1978). The SSP is the gold-standard for assessing quality of attachment in 11–15 month old infants (see Farnfield, et al. this issue). Infant attachment classification was blindly coded using both the DMM method and ABC+D methods. Around 20% of SSP videos were double coded using the ABC+D method, with a kappa of 0.78 on 3-way classification. Both ABC+D coders were trained to reliability in both the “ABC” and “D” classifications. DMM coding was performed by 3 reliable coders, and the kappa on 4-way classification ranged from 0.47 to 0.59. Crittenden reviewed SSPs where discrepancies existed, and the final classification was determined through conferencing.

Maternal depression

The presence of maternal depressive symptoms was assessed with the BDI, a 21-item screening instrument designed to assess the intensity of depression in clinical and normal patients during the previous two weeks. The BDI is demonstrates good clinical sensitivity with the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders—Fourth Edition (DSM–IV) diagnosis of depression (Coefficient Alpha = .92, Beck, 1996).

Personality disorder

Mothers were screened for the presence of a personality disorder using the PDQ-4+, a 99-item self-report questionnaire that assesses the ten personality disorders in DSM-IV. The questionnaire yields a total score, with higher scores of 50 or greater being highly suggestive of a SCID-II diagnosis of a personality disorder (Hyler, et al., 1992).

Maternal temperament

Maternal temperament was assessed using the ATQ, a 77-item self-report questionnaire reflecting the frequency of specified adult reactions to different situations. Four factors (effortful control, negative affect, extraversion/surgency, and orienting sensitivity) were measured (Rothbart, et al., 2000).

Infant temperament

Infant temperament was assessed with the IBQ-revised, a parent-report questionnaire to measure temperament in infants between the ages of 3 and 12 months of age on 14 dimensions; coefficient alphas range from 0.70–0.89 (Gartstein & Rothbart, 2003).

Parental stress

The mother’s experience of stress in the parenting role was assessed with the PSI (Abidin, 1995), a self-report clinical and research instrument for use with parents of children as young as one month of age. The PSI yields a total score, three domain scores (child, parent and total stress), and 15 subscales.

Infant development

The Bayley-III screening test was used to assess the developmental functioning of the infants at 14 months of age. The Bayley-III has been validated to assess the cognitive, language, and motor functioning of infants and young children (Bayley, 2006).

Univariate exploratory assessment of possible confounding variables on AAI and SSP classifications were performed with chi-squared-, t-, or rank-sum tests. We did not find confounding variables. Comparisons between infant SSP classifications in the DMM and ABC+D methods used the chi-squared test, Fischer’s Exact test, or Phi-statistic. Our overall hypothesis, with row by row and cell-by-cell predictions, was tested using the delta prediction statistic (Hildebrand, Laing, & Rosenthal, 1977). We used 1-tailed tests when the hypothesis stated a direction of effects.

The distributions of adult and infant attachment classifications are presented in Figure 1 and Table 1. The DMM-AAI classifications were dichotomized into Type B (secure) versus non-Type B (insecure) categories. These two attachment groups did not differ in demographics (age, race, income level, marital status, or IQ), adult temperament, screening measures of personality disorder risk or depression, reported parenting stress, or infant development. Although not statistically significant, insecure mothers tended to breastfeed less, spend more time separated from their infants, and have graduate degrees. Infants of insecure mothers scored significantly lower on “duration of orienting”, or extended time spent attending to and/or interacting with toys or other objects (df = 1, F = 6.493, p = 0.014) (Table 2).

Distribution of AAI-DMM Classifications

Attachment
TypeA5–6A3–4A1–2BC1–2C3–4C5–6A/CCannot
ClassifyDTotal
SSP-DMMN--852083--41a--49
%--16.310.240.816.46.1--8.22.0--
AAI-DMMN3982721340--57
%5.315.814.047.43.51.75.37.00--
SSP-ABC+DN----5336--------549
%----10.267.412.2--------10.2

Demographic characteristics of Secure (Type B) versus Insecure (Non-B) Mothers and their infants.

VariableSecure AAI(Type B)

(N= 23)

Insecure AAI(Non-B)

(N= 26)

Age of Mother, Visit 1, year ± SD27.7 ± 5.428.5 ± 4.2
Marital Status, N (%)
    Married17 (73.9%)17 (65.4%)
    Not married6 (26.1%)9 (34.6%)
Maternal Race, N (%)
    White13 (56.5%)17 (65.4%)
    Non-white10 (43.5%)9 (34.6%)
Income, N (%)
    <$30,000/year7 (30.4%)8 (33.3%)
    $30,001 – $70,0006 (26.1%)8 (33.3%)
    >$70,00010 (43.5%)8 (33.3%)
Maternal Education
    Some college6 (26.1%)5 (19.2%)
    College degree11 (47.8%)9 (34.6%)
    Graduate degree6 (26.1%)12 (46.2%)
Maternal IQ ± SD(N=22)(N=25)
(WTAR-predicted WAIS-III)109.8 ± 8.9109.8 ± 8.8
Personality Disorder Screen (PDQ) Visit 1, N (%)
    No positive screens11 (47.8%)9 (34.6%)
    1–2 positive screens8 (34.8%)9 (34.6%)
    >3 positive screens4 (17.4%)8 (30.8%)
Depression: BDI, Visit 1, N (%)(N=25)
    Minimal20 (87.0%)21 (84.0%)
    Mild3 (13.0%)2 (8.0%)
    Moderate02 (8.0%)
Parenting Stress Index (PSI) Visit 2, raw score ± SD(N=19)(N=22)
    Child Domain89.1 ± 12.493.9 ± 14.9
    Parent Domain108.5 ± 22.9115.5 ± 21.9
    Total Stress197.6 ± 28.7204.4 ± 37.1
Temperament of Mothers (ATQ), Visit 1, raw score ± SD(N=22)(N=25)
    Negative Affect3.9 ±0.63.9 ± 0.8
    Extraversion4.9 ± 0.64.8 ±0.7
    Effortful Control4.5 ± 0.74.7 ±0.8
    Orienting Sensitivity5.1 ± 0.75.1 ± 0.6
Infant Temperament (IBQ), Visit 1, raw score ± SD(N=21)(N=25)
    Approach5.7 ±0.95.3 ±1.4
    Vocal Reactivity5.2 ± 0.85.2 ± 0.9
    High intensity pleasure6.1 ± 0.66.1 ±0.6
    Activity Level4.9 ± 0.94.7 ± 0.8
    Perceptual Sensitivity4.7 ± 1.24.5 ± 1.1
    Distress to Limitations3.8 ± 0.73.9 ±0.7
    Fear2.6 ± 0.82.7 ± 1.3
    Low Intensity pleasure5.1 ± 1.55.3 ±0.8
    Cuddliness5.6 ±0.65.7 ± 0.7
    Duration of Orienting*4.6 ± 1.03.8 ± 1.0
Mother –Infant Separation, Visit 3, N (%)(N=22)(N=24)
    ≤20 hrs/week12 (54.5%)10 (41.7%)
    >20 hrs/week10 (45.5%)14 (58.3%)
Still Breastfeeding, N (%)
(N=20)(N=17)
    Visit 215 (75.0%)11 (64.7%)
(N=22)(N=25)
    Visit 311 (50.0%)12 (48.0%)
Child Development (Bayley), mean raw score ± SD(N=23)(N=25)
    Cognitive17.4 ± 1.417.7 ± 1.9
    Receptive Communication14.6 ± 2.314.1 ± 2.1
    Expressive Communication13.9 ± 1.913.6 ± 1.8
    Fine Motor15.9 ± 1.715.6 ± 2.1
    Gross Motor18.0 ± 1.217.9 ± 1.9

Hypothesis 1: Type B (secure) mothers will have infants with Type B (secure) attachment (i.e. classification matching)

Using the DMM secure-insecure comparison, mother and infant classifications matched 73.4 % of the time (Χ2 = 10.684, df = 1, κ = 0.463, p = 0.001). Four-way categorical comparison indicates less continuity within anxious attachment (see Table 3).

Association of prenatal AAI classifications with 14-month DMM-SSP classifications

DMM-AAI ClassificationDMM-SSP ClassificationType BType AType CType A/CTotal
Type BN1562023
% SSP Predicted by AAI65.2%26.1%8.7%0.0%100.0%
Adjusted residual3.30.0−2.2−2.2
Type AN537217
% SSP Predicted by AAI29.4%17.6%41.2%11.8%100%
Adjusted residual−1.2−1.02.30.3
Type CN04015
% SSP Predicted by AAI0.0%80.0%0.0%20.0%100.0%
Adjusted residual−2.02.9−1.30.8
Type A/CN00224
% SSP Predicted by AAI0.0%0.0%50.0%50.0%100.0%
Adjusted residual−1.7−1.31.42.7
TotalN201311549
% SSP Classifications40.8%26.5%22.4%10.2%100.0%

Hypothesis 2: Anxious maternal attachment will predict the opposite anxious infant pattern (i.e., classification inversion /meshing)

Of the mothers who were anxiously attached (Type A or C) with anxiously attached (Type A or C) infants (n=14), 7 out of 10 Type A mothers had Type C infants, and all 4 Type C mothers had Type A infants (Χ2 = 5.600, df = 1, p = 0.018, Fischer’s exact test: p = 0.035, one-tailed). A one-tailed test was employed because a direction of effect was being tested.

To test the full model of maternal security predicting infant security and maternal insecurity predicting the opposite pattern of infant insecurity, we used a delta prediction statistic. The overall hypothesis was significant (∆Ƥ = .38, p = .001, see Table 3).

Hypothesis 3: The DMM and ABC+D SSP classifications will be related

There was an association between the two classificatory methods (Χ2 = 7.886, df = 1, p = 0.005), although the measure of agreement was low (κ = 0.348; Table 4). There was greater agreement when classifying infants with secure attachment than with anxious. The ABC+D method identified substantially more Type B infants (67%) than the DMM Method (41%) (Figure 1).

Association between DMM-SSP classifications and ABC+D-SSP classifications.

ABC+D-SSP ClassificationDMM-SSP ClassificationType BType AType CType DTotal
Type BN1802020
% ABC+D-SSP predicted by DMM-SSP90.0%.0%10.0%.0%100.0%
Adjusted residual2.8−2.0−0.4−2.0
Type AN641213
% ABC+D-SSP predicted by DMM-SSP46.2%30.8%7.7%15.4%100.0%
Adjusted residual−1.92.9−0.60.7
Type CN503311
% ABC+D-SSP predicted by DMM-SSP45.5%0.0%27.3%27.3%100.0%
Adjusted residual−1.8−1.31.72.1
Type A/CN41005
% ABC+D-SSP predicted by DMM-SSP80.0%20.0%.0%.0%100.0%
Adjusted residual0.60.8−0.9−0.8
TotalN3356549
% ABC+D-SSP Classifications67.3%10.2%12.2%10.2%

Hypothesis 4: Adult attachment (DMM classification) will predict infant attachment using the ABC+D classification

The two-way (secure/insecure) match between mother’s AAIs and infants’ ABC+D-SSP classifications was 65.2%; this was not statistically significant (Χ2 = 0.089, df = 1, p = ns, see Table 5).

Associations between prenatal DMM-AAI classifications and 14-month ABC+D-SSP classifications.

ABC+D ClassificationAAI ClassificationType BType AType CType DTotal
Type BN1523323
% SSP Predicted by AAI65.2%8.7%13.0%13.0%100.0%
Adjusted residual−0.3−0.30.20.6
Type AN1222117
% SSP Predicted by AAI70.6%11.8%11.8%5.9%100.0%
Adjusted residual0.40.30.0−0.7
Type CN31105
% SSP Predicted by AAI60.0%20.0%20.7%0.0%8.2%
Adjusted residual.3−0.70.8−0.7
Type A/CN30014
% SSP Predicted by AAI75.%0.0%0.0%25.0%100.0%
Adjusted residual0.3−0.7−0.81.0
TotalN24174449
% SSP-ABC+D67.3%10.2%12.2%10.2%

Both DMM sets of classifications had substantially lower rates of security (41% DMM-SSP & 48% DMM-AAI) than the ABC+D-SSP classifications (67%) (Figure 1). The finding of lower rates of Type B with DMM methods is consistent across studies and ages and explains some of the lack of correspondence between the two methods (Crittenden, 2000b; Crittenden, Claussen, & Kozlowska, 2007). It does not, however, explain A versus C disagreements. Neither does the difference indicate which set of classifications is more useful.

The DMM classifications provide additional support for the notion of the intergenerational ‘transmission’ of attachment, but also suggest a more complex process for anxious attachment than simple continuity. Our findings are valuable because (1) the DMM method was not biased toward finding an association between the AAI and SSP and (2) our findings make some previous mismatches theoretically comprehensible. Specifically, we found that maternal security predicted infant security as has been previously demonstrated (van IJzendoorn, 1995). On the other hand, when mothers were anxiously attached, the association was between opposite patterns, as predicted by DMM theory. This may be adaptive to infants in that mothers using a Type A strategy tend to be under-responsive (A3,5) or over-responsive (A4, 6) in predictable ways; however, when their infants organize an affectively intense Type C strategy, the infants increase the probability of their mothers responding (A3, 5) or reducing their control (A4, 6). Conversely, when mothers use a Type C strategy of angry or helpless demands, their infants may benefit by employing a strategy of caring for (A3) or complying with (A4) their mothers. This finding may be particularly important in cases of risk, maltreatment, or psychopathology.

The mechanism by which maternal attachment influences infant attachment needs to be explored in greater detail. Our data suggest that infants do not directly acquire their mothers’ models and that the process of ‘transmission’ is quite complex, especially when infants are anxious about their mothers’ availability and responsiveness. Strathearn and his colleagues demonstrated that mothers in this study who were classified as Type B (DMM-AAI) showed greater activation of brain reward regions on viewing images of their own infants’ happy or sad faces than mothers classified as Type A (Strathearn et. al., 2009). This provides preliminary evidence that a mother’s own attachment experience in childhood may influence the development of reward and affiliation circuits in the brain that promote contingent and sensitive responses to her own infant’s cues. That is, a mother’s attachment experiences from her own childhood may shape neural circuits which influence how she perceives and responds to her infant’s cues one generation later. Mothers who were classified as Type A during pregnancy experienced their infants as less rewarding (as measured by fMRI activation) compared with mothers who had secure attachment. It is hypothesized that these differences in neural activation manifest as differences in interactive behavior between mothers and their infants. When mothers behave with sensitive responsiveness, infants respond in kind in a mutual dance of contingent and attuned dyadic shaping (Stern, 1985). But when mothers’ behaviour is incongruent, infants must adapt to that condition and may do so by employing complementary, rather than identical, strategies. This may maximize infants’ potential to survive.

The lack of association between the DMM-AAI classifications and the ABC+D-SSP classifications is best explained by differences in the coding systems. The DMM methods are more attuned to differences within anxious attachment and organize the behavioral patterns both strategically, i.e., functionally, and in terms of dimensional processes tied to information processing. Review of the distribution of the SSP-ABC+D classifications reveals a higher rate of security in the ABC+D classification than in the DMM classificatory systems. This apparent “overclassification” of infants as secure has been found in other studies (Crittenden, 2000a; Crittenden 2000b; Spieker & Crittenden, in press; Thompson & Raikes, 2003).

Implications for Clinical Intervention

The pattern of complementary anxious strategies has important significance when planning therapeutic interventions. Because maternal behavior derives from representations, (Bowlby, 1980; Stern-Brushweiler, & Stern, 1989), it is important to consider how the mother’s representations of her infant influence her behavior. Our fMRI data address this issue. Type A mothers appear to experience less natural reward when processing their own infants’ facial cues (as measured by fMRI activation) compared to mothers with secure attachment (Strathearn, et. al., 2009). As a result, the Type A mother’s typical pattern of unresponsiveness is likely to be enacted when confronted by her infant’s negative affect. This affective ‘mis-attunement’ (Stern, 1985) is a source of concern because the infant will have to accommodate to the mother, rather than vice versa. Given the infant’s limited maturation, short-cuts may need to be taken. These short-cuts take the form of behavioral rules (such as ‘scream louder’, ‘don’t cry’, ‘smile when you feel bad’) that have no basis in understanding or context. They function to improve the mother-infant relationship in the short-term, but may be maladaptive in later stages of development or in other contexts. Furthermore, the timing of interventions can play an important role in optimizing attunement between a mother and her infant. For many mothers, the birth of a child is associated with a period of psychic and inter-personal, family reorganization, which provides a unique opportunity for therapeutic intervention. In this period of time, whose duration is variable, the clinician has an opportunity to forge a new kind of therapeutic alliance and adopt a new treatment framework which transforms this period of fragility into an opportunity for growth and transformation (Stern, 1995).

Mis-attuned dyads can often benefit from intervention or psychological treatment. However, based on clinical experience, Crittenden has hypothesized that selecting the optimal treatment may depend upon which of the mother’s DRs is the basis for her behavior (Crittenden, 2008). For example, if the mother understands the baby’s state, but doesn’t have a repertoire of responses, a simple psycho-educational intervention may be most appropriate (cf. Crittenden, 1993). If she accurately perceives—but misconstrues—the meaning of her infant’s behavior, and can articulate her experience (‘he’s crying because he hates me’), a mother-baby intervention focusing on mentalization may assist her to reframe her understanding. If however, she misperceives her infant’s behavior, she may need intense personal work with her own psychological processes, i.e., psychotherapy (Crittenden, 2005). Failure to differentiate the psychological processes that lead to mothers’ mis-attuned responses can lead to mis-attuned interventions. Knowing the severity and psychological source of the mis-attunement seems crucial to attuning our response to the mother as we seek to create new possibilities for her relationship with her infant through our relationship with her.

Limitations and future directions

The data presented here elaborate our understanding of intergenerational patterning of attachment in which we found both a matching (continuity) effect for secure mothers and their infants and also a meshing (inversion) effect for anxious mothers and their infants. However, several limitations and caveats should be noted. First, our sample size was not large enough to test fully the association for the C and A/C patterns. In addition, the percentage of mothers with Type A attachments having children with Type C attachments is relatively uncommon, whereas the number of mothers with Type C attachment having children with avoidant (Type A) strategies is larger (Van IzJendoorn, 1995). Thus, our results, though significant in our small sample size, merit replication in a larger sample. Moreover, even in cases of security only half the variance was accounted for; larger samples and other assessments are needed to address the other co-occurring processes. In addition, at this time, the AAI’s have only been coded with the DMM system, making a full comparison of the two methods, as well as correspondence with infant attachment, not possible at present. Coding of our AAI transcripts with the Main and Goldwyn classificatory method is planned for the future. When completed, that set of data will be analyzed both for fMRI patterning and also relations with the DMM-AAI and both SSP sets of classifications. Further research is also needed to determine which attachment subtypes show continuity and under what conditions. In addition, research is needed to explore how different strategies evolve over the lifespan and how early strategies influence later behavioral organization.

A more basic issue is determining whether the DMM or ABC+D classificatory methods and systems better reflect mothers’ and infants’ strategies. Reclassification of our AAIs with the Main and Goldwyn method would permit comparison with the fMRI results. Unfortunately, this study cannot properly test the relative validity of the two sets of SSP classifications because we lack follow up data after the Strange Situation at 14 months. Given concern regarding the limited predictive validity of the ABC+D classifications (cf. Thompson & Raikes, 2003), cross-classification of a larger existing sample, including both normative and troubled dyads and with appropriate validating variables, would be timely.

The authors would like to gratefully acknowledge the invaluable contributions of Patricia Crittenden, Ph.D. who provided reviews, edits and helpful suggestions on earlier drafts of the manuscript. This research was supported by Award Numbers K21 HD43097 from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, and K12 HD41648 from the Baylor Child Health Research Center: Pediatrics Mentored Research Program.

Prachi E. Shah, University of Michigan-Ann Arbor.

Peter Fonagy, Anna Freud Institute.

Lane Strathearn, Baylor College of Medicine.

  • Abidin R. Parenting Stress Index, 3rd Ed Professional Manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessments Resources; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  • Ainsworth MDS, Blehar MC, Waters E, Wall S. Patterns of attachment: A Psychological Study of the Strange Situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1978. [Google Scholar]
  • Bayley N. Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  • Bowlby J. Attachment and Loss, Vol 1II (Loss) New York, NY: Basic Books; 1980. [Google Scholar]
  • Crittenden PM. Abusing, neglecting, problematic, and adequate dyads: Differentiating by patterns of interaction. Merrill Palmer Quarterly. 1981;27:1–18. [Google Scholar]
  • Crittenden PM. Social networks, quality of child rearing, and child development. Child Development. 1985;56:1299–1313. [Google Scholar]
  • Crittenden PM. Characteristics of neglectful parents: An information processing approach. Criminal Justice and Behavior. 1993;20:27–48. [Google Scholar]
  • Crittenden PM. Attaccamento in etâ adulta. L'approccio dinamico-maturativo alla Adult Attachment Interview. Edizione Italiana a cura di Graziella Fava Vizziello e Andrea Landini. Milano: Cortina; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  • Crittenden PM. A dynamic-maturational approach to continuity and change in pattern of attachment. In: Crittenden PM, Claussen AH, editors. The organization of attachment relationships: Maturation, culture, and context. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; 2000. pp. 343–357. [Google Scholar]
  • Crittenden PM. Der CARE-Index als Hilfsmittel für Früherkennung, Intervention und Forschung. Frühörderung interdisziplinär (early interdisciplinary intervention), Special issue: Bindungsorientierte Ansätze in der Praxis der Frühförderung. 2005;24:S.99–S.106. (Using the CARE-Index for screening, intervention, and research. (English on //www.patcrittenden.com.) [Google Scholar]
  • Crittenden PM. Raising parents: Attachment, parenting, and child safety. Collumpton, UK: 2008. p. Willan Publishing. [Google Scholar]
  • Crittenden PM, Ainsworth MDS. Child maltreatment and attachment theory. In: Cicchetti D, Carlson V, editors. Handbook of child maltreatment. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1989. pp. 432–463. [Google Scholar]
  • Crittenden PM, Klaussen AH. Adaptation to varied environments. In: Crittenden PM, Claussen AH, editors. The organization of attachment relationships: Maturation, culture, and context. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; 2000. pp. 234–248. [Google Scholar]
  • Crittenden PM, Claussen AH, Kozlowska K. Choosing a valid assessment of attachment for clinical use: A comparative study. Australia New Zealand Journal of Family Therapy. 2007;28:78–87. [Google Scholar]
  • Crittenden PM, Landini A. New York: Norton; The Adult Attachment Interview: Assessing psychological and interpersonal strategies. (in press) [Google Scholar]
  • Crittenden PM, Newman L. Comparing models of borderline personality disorder: Mothers’ experience, self-protective strategies, and dispositional representations. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry. (this issue) [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Egeland B, Sroufe A. Attachment and early maltreatment. Child Development. 1981;52:44–52. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Farnfield S, Hautamäki A, Nørbech P, Sahhar N. Dynamic-Maturational Model methods for assessing attachment. Journal of Clinical and Child Psychology. (this issue) [Google Scholar]
  • Fonagy P, Steele H, Steele M. Maternal Representations of Attachment During Pregnancy Predict the Organization of Infant-Mother Attachment at One Year of Age. Child Development. 1991;26:891–905. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Gartstein MA, Rothbart MK. Studying infant temperament via the Revised Infant Behavior Questionnaire. Infant Behavior and Development. 2003;26:64–86. [Google Scholar]
  • George C, Kaplan N, Main M. Adult Attachment Interview. Berkeley: Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley; 1985. [Google Scholar]
  • George C, Kaplan N, Main M. Adult Attachment Interview, 3rd Ed. Berkeley: Berkeley, Department of Psychology, University of California; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  • Hamilton CE. Continuity and Discontinuity of Attachment from Infancy through Adolescence. Child Development. 2000;71:690–694. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Hautamäki A, Hautamäki L, Neuvonen L, Maliniemi-Piispanen S. The transmission of attachment across three generations. European Journal of Developmental Psychology. (in press) [Google Scholar]
  • Hesse E. Discourse, memory, and the Adult Attachment Interview: A note with emphasis on the emerging Cannot Classify category. Infant Mental Health Journal. 1996;17:4–11. [Google Scholar]
  • Hildebrand DK, Laing LP, Rosenthal HR. Prediction analysis of cross classifications. New York: Wiley; 1977. [Google Scholar]
  • Hyler SE, Skodol AE, Oldham JM, Kellman HD, Doidge N. The validity of the PDQ-R: A replication in an out-patient sample. Comprehensive Psychiatry. 1992;33:s73–s77. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Ledoux J. The emotional brain: The mysterious underpinnings of emotional life. New York: Simon & Schuster; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  • Lewis M, Fearing C, Rosenthal S. Attachment over Time. Child Development. 2000;71:707–720. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Main M. Introduction to the Special Section on Attachment and Psychopathology: 2. Overview of the Field of Attachment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1996;64:237–243. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Main M. The Organized Categories of Infant, Child, and Adult Attachment: Flexible vs. Inflexible Attention Under Attachment Related Stress. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association. 2000;48:1055–1096. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Main M, Goldwyn R. Adult attachment scoring and classification system. Unpublished scoring manual. Berkeley: Department of Psychology, University of California; 1985–1994. [Google Scholar]
  • Main M, Kaplan N, Cassidy J. Security in Infancy, Childhood and Adulthood: A Move to the Level of Representation. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 1985;50:66–104. [Google Scholar]
  • Main M, Hesse E. Parent's Unresolved Traumatic Experiences Are Related to Infant Disorganized Attachment Status: Is Frightened and/or Frightening Parental Behavior the Linking Mechanism? In: Greenberg MT, Cicchetti D, Cummings EM, editors. Attachment in the preschool years. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1990. pp. 161–182. [Google Scholar]
  • Main M, Solomon J. Procedures for identifying infants as disorganized/disoriented during the Ainsworth Strange Situation. In: Greenberg MT, Cicchetti D, Cummings EM, editors. Attachment in the preschool years. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1990. pp. 121–160. [Google Scholar]
  • Radke-Yarrow M, Cummings EM, Kuczynski L, Chapman M. Patterns of attachment in two- and three-year-olds in normal families and families with parental depression. Child Development. 1985;56:884–893. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Rothbart MK, Ahadi SA, Evans DE. Temperament and Personality: Origins and Outcomes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2000;78:122–135. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Schacter DL, Tulving E. What are the memory systems of 1994? In: Schacter DL, Tulving E, editors. Memory systems. Cambridge, MA: Bradford; 1994. pp. 1–38. [Google Scholar]
  • Spieker S, Crittenden PM. Comparing the validity of two approaches to attachment theory: Disorganization versus danger-informed organization in the preschool years. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry. (in press) [Google Scholar]
  • Stern Daniel. The Interpersonal World of the Infant. New York: Basic Books; 1985. [Google Scholar]
  • Stern Daniel. The Motherhood Constellation. New York: Basic Books; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  • Stern-Bruschweiler N, Stern DN. A model for conceptualizing the role of the mother's representational world in various mother-infant therapies. Infant Mental Health Journal. 1989;10(3):142–156. [Google Scholar]
  • Strathearn L, Fonagy P, Amico JA, Montague PR. Adult attachment predicts mother's brain and oxytocin response to infant cues. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2009;34:2655–2666. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Strathearn L, Li J, Fonagy P, Montague PR. What's in a smile? Maternal brain responses to infant facial cues. Pediatrics. 2008;122:40–51. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Thompson RA, Raikes HA. Toward the Next Quarter Century: Conceptual and Methodological Challenges for Attachment Theory. Development and Psychopathology. 2003;15:691–718. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • van IJzendoorn MH. Adult Attachment Representations, Parental Responsiveness, and Infant Attachment: A Meta Analysis on the Predictive Validity of the Adult Attachment Interview. Psychological Bulletin. 1995;117:387–403. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Weinfeld NS, Sroufe LA, Egeland B. Attachment from Infancy to Early Adulthood in a High Risk Sample: Continuity, Discontinuity, and their Correlates. Child Development. 2000;71:695–702. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Postingan terbaru

LIHAT SEMUA